Violent Emotions: Shame and Rage in Marital Quarrels

Books

Suzanne M. Retzinger

  • Citations
  • Add to My List
  • Text Size

  • Chapters
  • Front Matter
  • Back Matter
  • Subject Index
  • Copyright

    View Copyright Page

    Preface

    Mary AnneFitzpatrick

    The main reason for reading a preface is to find out what the book has to say. Suzanne Retzinger has done such a fine job of presenting her theory and research that a detailed description by me of this book would be redundant. Consequently, after a cursory treatment of the main points of Dr. Retzinger's argument, I will try to indicate the importance of this book by placing it within the context of the work concerned with communication in the family across a number of social science disciplines. These last are fields with which I am familiar, but they are relevant beyond that fact. A communication theory of the family must account for emotional processes, and Dr. Retzinger's research moves the field in an important direction in that regard.

    According to Dr. Retzinger, connection with others is the primary motive in human behavior and is accomplished through communication. Central to her theory is the idea that social connections or bonds between people are at risk in all encounters: If they are not being built, maintained, or repaired, they are being damaged. Within her framework of human sociability, an important source of conflict and aggression between intimates becomes reactions to lapses in important social bonds. A perceived attack by one partner on the bond between partners causes rage and shame and hence conflict escalation.

    Dr. Retzinger presents a powerful theory of emotional processes and conflict escalation in intimate relationships. In testing her theory of protracted conflict, she employs complex verbal and nonverbal coding schemes, identifies specific emotions within the context of marital quarrels, and isolates recurring patterns preceding the escalation of a quarrel. She provides exemplars of how this theory works through the intensive analysis of segments of conflict exchanges in four couples. The reader will come to know Rosie and James, David and Colleen, Roxanne and Brian, and Randy and Karin very well by the end of this book. I suspect that many researchers, teachers, and therapists will turn to the vivid descriptions and transcripts the author provides to illustrate important points about communication in intimate relationships.

    If Dr. Retzinger had accomplished only these feats in her book, it would be an important addition to the literature. She has, however, done much more. I hope to show the extent of her contribution by discussing the fault lines dividing the research on family process, the central role of affect in understanding conflict dynamics, and how this theory can benefit other major lines of investigation in the family area.

    The Epistemological Fault Lines

    The study of marital and family interaction cuts across disciplinary boundaries. Researchers are housed in a variety of disciplines, including communication, psychiatry, social work, sociology, and clinical, social, and developmental psychology. Each discipline approaches the study of family interaction somewhat differently. Some of the differences come from the weights that each discipline assigns to the various levels of analysis from which an examination of the family can proceed (Fitzpatrick & Wamboldt, 1990); some come from the quantitative versus qualitative fault line that splits family communication researchers.

    This book and the way Dr. Retzinger approaches her research may keep the fault line from becoming a chasm. She heavily references and relies on both quantitative and qualitative work to build her argument, displays great sensitivity about how the “other side” may view the importance of case studies, and is extraordinarily careful in documenting her arguments about sequence and pattern in these dialogues. In other words, within a conversation, Dr. Retzinger compares the presence or absence of shame and rage and its relationship to escalation, and justifies her conclusions with empirical evidence. It is up to the reader to decide the degree to which the method of proof used in this book defends against alternative explanations. But the same holds true for the reader of a piece of communication science research in family interaction (Jacobs, 1988).

    Dr. Retzinger and I are on opposite sides of this epistemological fault line; whereas she may be considered a discourse analyst who uses a case study method, with its focus on intensive analysis and in-depth exemplars, I am a communication scientist who studies family communication processes by employing large-scale data sets and quantitative techniques to examine sequence and pattern in couple communication. According to the usual logic of these positions, the methods of argument and the standards of proof differ so radically that little can be learned from the writing generated on the other side of the fault line. Like children at parallel play, work on family dynamics proceeds along similar tracks within each camp, yet is rarely acknowledged outside of the group. Panel discussions at academic conventions sometimes give lip service to how much we all have to learn from one another. The political fact, however, is that researchers and theorists working in what might be called discourse-analytic approaches versus communication science approaches to communication in the family rarely read or cite each other's work, appear in each other's edited volumes, or review each other's books.

    As a communication scientist, I see Dr. Retzinger operating within the context of discovery in that she has carefully outlined a theory that can be (and is) operationalized and tested. The assumptions behind her theory as well as her propositions are unambiguously presented to the reader. Communication scientists, working within a context of justification, may want to demonstrate the relative frequency of occurrence or the situational variability of the emotional patterns that Dr. Retzinger has discerned in her four conversations. Indeed, I believe that important research in communication science can be generated from the theory presented by Dr. Retzinger on how couples and other intimates escalate conflict by attacking the social bonds. Those workers in communication science interested in third-party intervention and mediation may find exactly the theoretical stance they need on conflict in intimate relationships in Dr. Retzinger's theory (Donohue, Lyles, & Rogan, 1989).

    The Role of Affect in Intimate Conflict

    Few would argue with the commonsense notion that emotion in families and close relationships is an important area of study. Many would be surprised to find that until recently little attention has been paid to this area of study. Within the last decade, however, given the work of a number of fine investigators, it has become the accepted view that the emotional climate of a distressed and conflict-ridden marriage includes both more negative affect and more reciprocation of negative affect than that of a nondistressed marriage. This finding has been replicated in different laboratories and cultures and appears to show strong cross-situational consistency within a given couple. In addition, the causal effect of such negative affect cycles has been demonstrated in longitudinal designs (see decade review by Noller & Fitzpatrick, 1990). If this is the accepted view, what information do we gain about affect by reading the work of Dr. Retzinger?

    Two answers to this question immediately spring to mind. The most obvious answer is that the strength of this theory is that it unpacks the construct of negative affect so that we may examine it more closely. Rage and shame are given central theoretical focus as the causal mechanisms responsible for marital conflict escalation. Anger alone is not the villain, but the accompanying shame that comes with an attack on the marital bond. A less obvious, yet far more important, answer is that the author presents an interactional theory of affect (also see Gottman, 1990). Family communication theorists often decry the scarcity of relational-level terms in both naive language and scientific writing. Within the theoretical tradition that attempts to keep concepts and inferences at the relational level, and thus rejects individual-motivational terms, affect has long been suspect (Raush, Greif, & Nugent, 1979). Within this decade, negative affect reciprocity has joined this construct class, yet still other relational-level affect descriptors for interaction are needed. In a field still weak in language to describe process, Dr. Retzinger introduces social emotions or emotions that operate to regulate the bond between people in order to ensure the survival of connections between intimates.

    Aside from the theoretical importance of this language, it has pragmatic value as well. By the end of this book, Dr. Retzinger is able to give pragmatically oriented readers advice about short-circuiting dysfunctional communication patterns. Rather than making generic statements about being more positive and less negative, she shows the reader actual ways to derail the rage-shame interact, and accompanying conflict escalation, either within one's own interaction or when observing that of a third party.

    Relationship to other Research

    One of the major ways that scientific research is judged is the degree to which it complements other research endeavors. The working assumption for those who research intimate relationships is that embedded patterns of interaction between partners cannot be successfully hidden but are revealed through a close examination of how couples communicate. Dr. Retzinger's research is clearly within this tradition: It should set to rest the doubts anyone might have about the willingness and ability of couples to engage in conflict with one another while being videotaped in a scientist's laboratory (I refer skeptics to Karin and Randy).

    In reading the intensive analysis of the dialogues of Retzinger's couples, I was struck by the similarity of the conflict patterns she uncovered to those I have seen in my laboratory (Fitzpatrick, 1988, 1991). From both the presentation of psychological information about the couples and their standard dialogic patterns of engagement and avoidance of marital conflicts, I would like to speculate as to the definition of the marriage held by each couple. The definition that each couple holds concerning their marriage incorporates three dimensions: ideology (e.g., relational beliefs, values, and standards), interdependence (e.g., degree of connectedness), and expressivity (e.g., views on conflict avoidance/engagement). I would argue that the definition that couples hold about their marriage is one way to describe in detail the kind of bond that couples have. This description may help to predict a priori which messages would be more likely to be an assault on the bond for given sets of couples and, considering the degree to which this theory complements my own work, will allow me to make more substantive and theoretical statements about conflict escalation in various types of marriages (for an attempt, see Burrell & Fitzpatrick, 1990).

    Rosie and James have conventional marital and family values, yet also they privilege their individual freedoms over the maintenance of the relationship. This couple is not very companionate, and they share little with each other, trying to maintain a psychological distance in their relationship. Rosie and James might describe their communication with each other as persuasive and assertive, yet they collude in avoiding open conflict. One partner may display outright hostility yet retreat quickly if the partner disagrees. In my terminology, Rosie and James are Separates.

    Roxanne and Brian share nonconventional values about relationships and family life, yet are very open and companionable with one another. This couple is constantly renegotiating their roles, and disagreements are a fundamental part of their relationship. Because they tend to engage in conflict very openly, Roxanne and Brian are Independents.

    In my terminology, David and Colleen are a mixed type, in that he appears to have an independent view of the marriage whereas she has a separate definition. Mixed types are noted for their disagreement on fundamental issues in the relationship as well as their different ideologies and views about connectedness. David and Colleen are Independents/Separates.

    Although the definitions of their marriages held by the first three couples seem to fall clearly into patterns I have previously uncovered, I am loath to speculate about Randy and Karin. Not only is there less information given about the values and levels of interdependence in this marriage, but this couple seems to be in severe distress after the loss of two children. Whereas the other three couples enacted protracted yet manageable conflict escalation, the nature of the conflict interaction in Randy and Karin's marriage is severe and intense. This couple may be an Independent couple who, although previously able to control their angry exchanges, have now escalated their conflict out of control.

    Concern for saving the face of the partner and preserving the bond in the marriage is a major feature of the final major type of couple I have isolated in my research, the Traditionals. Traditionals do not appear in this book about rage and shame in the escalation of marital quarrels. It may be that Traditional couples use the conflict deescalation techniques suggested by Dr. Retzinger in the final section.

    Conclusion

    The theory and research presented in this book pass what I think of as David Reiss's (1981) three-point heuristic test of a model of family processes. To have lasting effects, a work on family process must be found to be (a) plausible and familiar, (b) robust enough to stand revision and tinkering, and (c) capable, with some additional work, of greatly expanding our understanding of family process. Dr. Retzinger's model of conflict escalation through the emotional displays of rage and shame has this heuristic power.

    Acknowledgments

    This book proposes a theory and method for understanding conflict in relationships. Primary emphasis is placed on the importance of the social bond; lapses in the bond are precursors for conflict. Alienation and emotion usually precede disruption. Although it is common knowledge that anger is involved in conflict, the important role that shame plays in perpetuating anger is not as well understood. I use the information from many fields in a microscopic approach to understanding conflict

    Writing this book took several years of hard work and help from many people: Financial support was provided by the Patent Fund and the Humanities/Social Science Research Grant at the University of California, Santa Barbara, and by Sarah Scheff.

    Parts of this book were based on three of my articles: “The Role of Shame in Marital conflict,” in Perspectives on Social Problems (parts of Chapters 47); “Shame, Anger, and Conflict: Case Study of Emotional Violence,” in Journal of Family Violence (part of Chapter 7); and “Shame-Rage Spirals: Videotape Studies,” in H. B. Lewis (Ed.), The Role of Shame in Symptom Formation (1987). Permission to reprint this material is gratefully acknowledged.

    I thank Lawrence Erlbaum Associates for permission to reprint tables from the article “Shame and Guilt in Neurosis,” by H. B. Lewis; and W. H. Freeman for permission to reprint a figure from Close Relationships, edited by Harold H. Kelly et al.

    The following persons gave helpful comments: Gale Miller (Perspectives on Social Problems) and Michel Hernsen (Journal of Family Violence). I am indebted to Robert Levenson for granting permission to use one of his videotapes for this study.

    Many thanks to Bert Adams for his comments on an earlier draft; Anthony Giddens for his long, detailed review; and Don Brown. Mary Ann Fitzpatrick and John Braithwaite have also been most helpful. Thanks go also to Lori Terry for her secretarial assistance and to D. J. McLaren, who provided construction of Figures 2.1, 2.2, and 2.6. I am grateful to Catherine Welles for her conversations that contributed to Chapter 2, on social distance and boundaries. I also thank Melvin Lansky for his comments, clarification of ideas, and continued support.

    I am indebted to those who have been a source of support throughout this project: Richard Applebaum, John Baldwin, and Dorothy Kreuger. Each helped by offering emotional and intellectual support, editing, comments, varying perspectives, and suggestions.

    I am grateful to my children, Jennifer, Thomas, and Lydia, who have provided a haven of retreat, kept me in perspective, and were patient and responsible throughout this project. I have learned an immense amount about human relationships from them.

    My debt to the late Helen B. Lewis cannot be expressed easily. She was my adviser and role model; her support and loving care will be difficult to match. Her Psychic War in Men and Women provided the seed for this book; I express my sorrow for the loss of a great woman. I extend my gratitude to her husband, Naphtali Lewis, for his continued encouragement.

    My greatest debt is to my friend and colleague Thomas Scheff; we explored ideas about the social bond and emotions together. His insight into human nature provided a point of departure; his tolerance made my exploration possible, allowing me to try out new ideas. He has relentlessly read and reread earlier versions of this book and has been my most important editor and critic. He has been an unending source of emotional and intellectual support and encouragement. I extend my most abundant appreciation.

    Finally, I wish to thank the couples who volunteered to be a part of this study. They are true pioneers who enabled me to map a passage into an unexplored territory

    Suzanne M.Retzinger

    Introduction

    The Godhead is broken like bread. We are the pieces. (“Herman Melville,” W. H. Auden)

    Randy and Karin, like many couples, have been arguing about the same topic for years; they can't seem to connect with each other:

    10:13.50 K: ya but the support doesn't come in telling me not what to eat but realizing that I have some problems an I'm internalizing them is where the support an YOU should come in

    23.34 R: ya but I can't get into your mind

    Although the primary topic of their argument is the weight Karin has gained, they go from topic to topic. They are alienated and feel hopeless in the situation; they blame each other for the problems:

    13:53.00 K: … that sex isn't the greatest, could any of it be your fault?

    R: NO

    56.62 K: NONE of it?

    R: uhuh

    K: none of it in BED was your fault, it was all my fault that it wasn't the greatest (1.8) you see what you're saying

    R: ya I do () and that's what I'm saying

    14:10.00 K: wel-I mean but lets be honest about it () I'm THAT horrible in bed and you're that exciting () is that what you're saying

    R: umhmm

    Blame only makes the situation worse. Change of topic does not get them any further in resolving their issues, but leads them further into entrenchment; they move to another topic, and the vehemence between them rapidly increases. To observers their topics often seem trivial:

    54.40 R: … I mean it used to be that you had some actual lips () NOW::you're there's-ther there's very

    17:01.31 K: (interrupting R) [you don't have any upper lip]

    R: little definition

    K: don't talk about my lips

    Randy and Karin might be any of us. Although the topic may be different, many couples can recognize the beat of the drums in their own relationships. The topic could be body weight, money, sex, the division of household duties, or the way the kids are raised. Although topics may change, giving the appearance of a new quarrel, the beat goes on. Some couples end in divorce, some live with conflict and violence, others lead lives of quiet desperation; some actually resolve their conflict.

    The questions I raise are these: How is it that the same quarrel can continue for years on end, even with great effort to resolve it? What are the driving forces beneath the quarrel that give seemingly trivial topics a powerful life of their own? What roles do emotions play? How can conflict be self-perpetuating?

    Although this book is about couples, it may also have implications for other forms of conflict. Conflict is a fundamental problem facing our society. It is clear that conflict is endemic, as suggested by even the most casual glance at present world conditions. Evidence for conflict can be found in all areas of modern civilization; so little seems to change—wars rage on, violence continues among family members, races, classes, and religions. All the expert systems and advanced abstract knowledge have not seemed to help.

    Dahrendorf (1965) implies that conflict is a basic human element when he argues that “wherever we find human societies there is conflict. Societies … do not differ as to the presence or absence of conflict, but rather to the degree of violence and intensity” (p. 171). Simmel (1955) suggests that “a certain amount of discord, inner divergence and outer controversy, is organically tied up with the very elements that ultimately hold the group together” (p. 18); conflict can be important for group cohesion.

    If one assumes an innate propensity for conflict, this assumption might lead to the belief that conflict is a predominant motive of human behavior. On the other hand, if human beings are thought to be fundamentally social creatures, maintenance of social contact would be primary. One question to ask is, If conflict is fundamental to human societies, what is it that holds the group together?

    Marx (1844/1964) and Simmel (1955) suggest the underlying function of conflict—restoration of social bonds and group unity. The implication of this perspective is that human behavior is not primarily conflictful, competitive, or aggressive, but rather social; conflict is an attempt to restore bonds—the social bond being the glue that holds the group together.

    Marx began his analysis with concern for communal bonds, assuming connectedness (solidarity) among its members. In his view, communal bonds were being replaced by alienation in the Industrial Revolution; with the emergence of modern industrial society, community had declined. Marx's writings represent a plea for community, and the hope that eventually conflict would restore community. Simmel (1955) takes a similar view: “Separation does not follow from conflict but, on the contrary, conflict from separation” (p. 47). Although both Marx and Simmel propose that the function of conflict is to restore group cohesion or unity, other theories are permeated with the assumption that aggression and conflict are primary.

    A rapidly changing world can lead to breakdown in community (bonding between identified persons). Community is replaced by society—where persons are “arbitrarily and artificially united by promise and contract” (Scheler, 1961, p. 166). Where relationships once were immediate, with rapid changes in time and space, they are becoming increasingly more abstract and distant (Giddens, 1989). Social relationships are being lifted out of the present. In an alienated society there is danger of excessive conformity to symbolic values in an attempt to reestablish community. In the process of overconformity, certain aspects of the self are in danger of being lost; the result can be alienation from self.

    Recent studies by Lewis (1976), Bowlby (1988), Ainsworth (1989), and others suggest that sociability and affectionateness are primary to bonding; conflict seems to arise only under specific conditions, such as thwarts, threats, or damage to social bonds (i.e., loss of face). A glance at the earliest behavior of human beings finds clutching and clinging rather than fight or flight. Sociability and affectionateness seem to occur prior to aggression; connection with others can be a goal of even conflictful behavior.

    I chose to study conflict escalation in marriages for several reasons. First, broken family bonds can be one of the most intense sources of conflict. For example, in some child custody disputes, two otherwise rational people seem to become temporarily insane. Each partner becomes violently reactive to the slightest gesture of the other; the two cannot disentangle themselves emotionally from each other—the level of conflict can increase virtually without limit. Another reason to study marriages is that a precise method can be used that captures moment-by-moment escalation: videotapes of actual quarrels.

    The integration of diverse approaches to conflict is the foundation for this book: couples conflict, community conflict, communication theories, large-scale warfare. Conflict theorists have discussed the importance of the social bond, but recent work on marital and family conflict, based primarily on atheoretical observations, has virtually ignored this aspect of human behavior. Few have dealt with sequences of events that occur during escalation.

    Relationships among social bonds, shame, and conflict are proposed. Protracted conflict is marked by social-emotional separation and unacknowledged shame, leading to anger, which in turn is expressed with disrespect, which leads to further separation, and so on. The role of hidden alienation and shame as the source of repetitious cycles of conflict is investigated.

    This book is divided into three parts that present theory, case studies, and conclusions. Part I reviews studies from the different disciplines, and provides a background for a new theory (Chapter 2). An integrative method (Chapter 3) has implications for theory building, research, and practice; it is applied to four cases.

    Chapter 1 covers theories and research from large-scale conflict, marital disputes, and communication processes. Sociological theories provide a wide-angle view of basic variables that underlie conflict. However, they deal mainly with abstract explanations; few provide concrete empirical examples.

    Unlike their sociological cousin, recent psychological approaches to marital conflict deal with emotions and communication patterns between persons. Although they deal with emotions, very few address specific emotions or the function of emotion during conflict; emotions are usually clumped into two categories, positive and negative. In studies that do specify emotions, behavior and emotion are often confused. The implications of such groupings are important for understanding conflict.

    Work on marital conflict has also dealt with concrete behavioral patterns, but lacks a theoretical backdrop—it does not tie into any of the existing theories of conflict, and does not build theory from the findings. Rather than dealing with conflict per se, these studies are concerned primarily with the comparison of functional and dysfunctional marriages—that is, understanding what distinguishes distressed from nondistressed, satisfied from dissatisfied. Few have attempted to expand the traditional theories or to spell out dynamics of escalation. Much can be learned from integrating findings from various approaches.

    Another problem with many of the conflict studies is that only aggregate data have been used. While useful in describing patterns of conflict and prevalence, they lose some of the richness found in qualitative studies; aggregate studies do not lend themselves to a dynamic theory of conflict escalation.

    There are few theories of conflict escalation compared with studies of conflict in marriage; some mention escalation in passing and, like other research on conflict, touch only the surface of the problem. Escalation as yet has not been adequately explained.

    Chapter 2 outlines a theory of conflict. Social bonds are vital to community as well as to individual relationships. Conflict is discussed in regard to threatened and broken bonds. I suggest that the emotion of shame plays a particularly important role in the structure and process of bonding, and therefore in conflict. A new theory of escalation is described: Escalation of conflict occurs when there is alienation and shame is evoked but not acknowledged.

    Chapter 3 develops a method that has implications not only for understanding escalation, but also for resolution of conflict (i.e., repair of the bond). It integrates visual, verbal, and paralinguistic methods. Applied intensively to case material, this method may help to advance our knowledge of conflict.

    Case studies with sequential analysis are not without limitations. They are time-consuming and expensive to conduct. Intensive investigations also do not provide information about prevalence, but gain in advancing knowledge for understanding sequences of behavior that would otherwise remain invisible. Intensive research can reveal nuances of behavior that hide underlying similarities, and lends itself well to application to practical problems.

    Part II presents four case studies of marital quarrels. Each couple is introduced with an explanation of their background. Each couple is unique, and on the surface they all look very different. Hidden beneath surface differences are similarities, however. As each couple quarrels, escalation is observed, moment by moment. Underlying the vast differences, similarities emerge—patterns that lead to escalation: the role of unacknowledged alienation and shame in the generation of anger or silence. Because of the complex and detailed nature of the analysis, only a short segment of each couple's argument is analyzed in detail.

    Part III addresses repairing the bond and presents some propositions and conclusions. Chapter 8 deals specifically with repair and implications for resolution. The dynamics of resolution are based on an inverse view of the new theory and findings on conflict, as well as on observations of actual instances of deescalation. Practical implications are also reviewed. The last chapter reviews patterns that emerge from the four cases, develops propositions, and discusses the ambiguity of communication and problems of translation.

  • Appendix

    Rosie and James

    David and Colleen

    Roxanne and Brian

    Randy and Karin

    References

    Adamson, L., Als, H., Tronick, E., & Brazelton, T. B. (1977). The development of social reciprocity between a sighted infant and her blind parents. Journal of American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 16, 194–207. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0002-7138%2809%2960036-4
    Adamson, L., & Bakeman, R. (1982). Affectivity and reference: Concepts, methods, and techniques in the study of communication development of 6-to-18-month-old infants. In T.Field & A.Fogel (Eds.), Emotion and early interaction. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
    Ainsworth, M. (1989). Attachment beyond infancy. American Psychologist, 44, 709–716. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.44.4.709
    Ainsworth, M., Bell, S., & Stayton, D. (1974). Infant-mother attachment social development: “Socialization” as a product of reciprocal responsiveness to signals. In M.Richards (Ed.), The integration of the child into a social world. New York: Cambridge University Press.
    Allport, G. (1937). Personality: A psychological interpretation. New York: Henry Holt.
    Archer, D., & Akert, R. M. (1977). Words and everything else: Verbal and nonverbal cues in social interpretation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 35, 443–449. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.35.6.443
    Averill, J. R. (1982). Anger and aggression: An essay on emotion. New York: Springer-Verlag. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-5743-1
    Bach, G. R., & Wydens, P. (1968). The intimate enemy. New York: Morrow.
    Bakan, D. (1966). The duality of human existence. Chicago: Rand McNally.
    Bales, R. (1950). Interaction process analysis: A method for the study of small groups. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
    Barnett, L. R., & Nietzel, M. T. (1979). Relationship of instrumental and affectional behaviors and self-esteem to marital satisfaction in distressed and non-distressed couples. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 47, 946–957. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.47.5.946
    Barrett, K., & Campos, J. (1987). Perspectives on emotional development II: A functional approach to emotions. In J. D.Osofsky (Ed.), Handbook of infant development (pp. 555–578). New York: John Wiley.
    Bateson, G. (1972). Steps to an ecology of mind. New York: Ballantine.
    Bateson, G., Jackson, D., Haley, J., & Weakland, J. (1956). Toward a theory of schizophrenia. Behavioral Science, 1, 251–264. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/bs.3830010402
    Baxter, I., (1988). A dialectical perspective on communication strategies in relationship development. In S.Duck (Ed.), Handbook of personal relationships (pp. 257–273). New York: John Wiley.
    Beck, A. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorder. New York: International University Press.
    Benedict, R. (1946). The chrysanthemum and the sword. New York: Houghton Mifflin.
    Berkowitz, L. (1983). Aversively stimulated aggression: Some parallels and differences in research with animals and humans. American Psychologist, 38, 1135–1160. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.38.11.1135
    Berscheid, E. (1983). Emotion. In H.Kelley, E.Berscheid, A.Christensen, J.Harvey, T.Huston, G.Levinger, L.Peplau, & D.Peterson (Eds.), Close relationships. New York: W. H. Freeman.
    Birchler, G. R., Weiss, R. L., & Vincent, J. P. (1975). Multimethod analysis of social reinforcement exchange between maritally distressed and nondistressed spouse and stranger dyads, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 349–360. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0076280
    Blumer, H. (1936). Social attitudes and nonsymbolic interaction. Journal of Educational Sociology, 9, 515–523. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2262360
    Boulding, K. (1962). Conflict and defense: A general theory. New York: Harper.
    Bowen, M (1978). Family therapy in clinical practice. New York: Jason Aronson.
    Bowlby, J. (1963). Pathological mourning and childhood mourning. Journal of American Psychoanalytic Association, 11, 500–541. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/000306516301100303
    Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss (Vol. 1). New York: Basic Books.
    Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss (Vol. 2). New York: Basic Books.
    Bowlby, J. (1988). A secure base: Parent-child attachment and healthy human development. New York: Basic Books.
    Bradshaw, J. (1988). Healing the shame that binds you. Dearfield Beach, FL: Health Communications.
    Braithwaite, J. (1988). Crime, shame and reintegration. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
    Brazelton, T. B. (1982). Joint regulation of neonate-parent interaction. In E.Tronick (Ed.), Social exchange in infancy. Baltimore: University Park Press.
    Brazelton, T. B., Koslowski, B. V., & Main, M. (1974). The origins of reciprocity: Early mother-infant interaction. In M.Lewis & L.Rosenblum (Eds.), The effect of the infant on its caregiver. New York: John Wiley.
    Brockner, J., & Rubin, J. Z. (1985). Entrapment in escalating conflicts. New York: Springer-Verlag. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-5072-2
    Brown, D. E. (1991). Human universals. New York: McGraw-Hill.
    Buck, R. (1984). The communication of emotion. New York: Guilford.
    Burgess, E., & Cottrell, L. (1939). Predicting success and failure in marriage. New York: Prentice-Hall.
    Burrell, N., & Fitzpatrick, M. A. (1990). Psychological reality of marital conflict. In D.Cahn (Ed.), Intimates in conflict (pp. 167–185). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
    Campos, J., & Stenberg, C. (1981). Perception, appraisal and emotion: The onset of social referencing. In M.Lamb & L.Sherrod (Eds.), Infant social cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
    Cavanaugh, E. (1989). Understanding shame. Minneapolis: Johnson Institute.
    Coleman, J. (1957). Community conflict. New York: Free Press.
    Collins, R. (1975). Conflict sociology: Towards an explanatory science. New York: Academic.
    Condon, W. S., & Ogston, W. D. (1971). Speech and body motion synchrony of the speaker-hearer. In D. L.Horton & J. J.Jenkins (Eds.), Perception of language. Columbus, OH: Charles E. Merrill.
    Cooley, C. H. (1962). Social organization. New York: Schocken. (Original work published 1909)
    Cooley, C. H. (1964). Human nature and the social order. New York: Schocken. (Original work published 1902)
    Coser, L. A. (1956). The functions of social conflict. New York: Free Press.
    Cottle, T. J. (1980). Children's secrets. Garden City, NY: Anchor.
    Cuber, J. F., & Harroff, P. B. (1965). Sex and the significant Americans. Baltimore: Pelican.
    Dahrendorf, R. (1965). Gesellshaft und demokratie in Deutschland. Munich: Piper Verlag.
    Darwin, C. (1965). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. London: John Murray. (Original work published 1872)
    DeCasper, A., & Fifer, W. (1981). Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mothers' voices. Science, 208, 1174–1176. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.7375928
    Deutsch, M. (1969). Conflicts: Productive and destructive. Journal of Social Issues, 25, 7–41. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1969.tb02576.x
    Dewey, J. (1922). Human nature and conduct. New York: Modern Library.
    Dewey, J. (1958). Experience and human nature. New York: Dover. (Original work published 1925)
    Dodds, E. (1951). The Greeks and the irrational. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    Donohue, W., Lyles, J., & Rogan, R. (1989). Issue development in divorce mediation. Mediation Quarterly, 24, 19–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/crq.39019892404
    Duck, S. (Ed.). (1988). Handbook of personal relationships. New York: John Wiley.
    Duncan, S., Jr., & Fiske, D. W. (1977). Face-to-face interaction: Research, method and theory. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
    Durkheim, E. (1964). The division of labor in society. New York: Free Press. (Original work published 1893)
    Durkheim, E. (1966). Suicide. New York: Free Press. (Original work published 1897)
    Edelmann, R. J., Asendorf, J., Contarello, A., Zammuner, V., et al. (1989). Self-reported expression of embarrassment in five European cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 20, 357–371. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0022022189204002
    Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. (1975). Unmasking the face. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
    Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. (1978). Facial action coding system. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.
    Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. (1982). Felt, false and miserable smiles, fournal of Non-verbal Behavior, 6, 238–252. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00987191
    Ellis, H. (1900). The evolution of modesty. Philadelphia: F. A. Davis.
    Erikson, E. H. (1963). Childhood and society. New York: W. W. Norton.
    Feshbach, S. (1956). The catharsis hypothesis and some consequences of interaction with aggression and neutral play objects. Journal of Personality, 24, 449–462. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1956.tb01281.x
    Field, T., Woodson, R., & Cohen, D. (1982). Discrimination and imitation of facial expression by neonates. Science, 218, 179–181. http://dx.doi.org/10.1126/science.7123230
    Fitzpatrick, M. A. (1988). Between husbands and wives: Communication in marriage. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
    Fitzpatrick, M. A. (in press). Sex differences in marital conflict: Social psychological verse cognitive explanations. Text.
    Fitzpatrick, M. A., & Wamboldt, F. (1990). Where all is said and done: Toward an integration of intrapersonal and interpersonal models of marital and family communication. Communication Research, 17, 421–431. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/009365090017004001
    Folger, J., & Poole, M. (1984). Working through conflict. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman.
    Fossum, M., & Mason, M. (1986). Facing shame: Families in recovery. New York: W. W. Norton.
    Freud, S. (1959). Inhibitions, symptoms, and anxiety. In S.Freud, Standard edition (Vol. 20, pp. 87–172). London: Hogarth. (Original work published 1926)
    Freud, S., & Breuer, J. (1961). Studies on hysteria. New York: Avon. (Original work published 1896)
    Funkenstein, D. H., King, S., & Drolette, M. (1957). Mastery of stress. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    Gaylin, W. (1979). Feelings. New York: Harper & Row.
    Gaylin, W. (1984). The rage within: Anger in modern life. New York: Simon & Schuster.
    Gaylin, W., & Person, E. (1988). Passionate attachments. New York: Free Press.
    Gelles, R. J. (1987). Family violence. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
    Gelles, R. J., & Straus, M. A. (1979a). Determinants of violence in the family: Toward a theoretical integration. In W.Burr, R.Hill, F. I.Nye, & I.Reiss (Eds.), Contemporary theories about the family. New York: Free Press.
    Gelles, R. J., & Straus, M. A. (1979b). Violence in the American family. Journal of Social Issues, 35, 15–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1979.tb00799.x
    Giddens, A. (1989). Paper presented at a colloquium, University of California, Santa Barbara, Department of Sociology.
    Goffman, E. (1959). The presentation of self in everyday life. Garden City: Doubleday.
    Goffman, E. (1967). Interaction ritual. Garden City, NY: Anchor.
    Gottman, J. M. (1979). Marital interaction. New York: Academic Press.
    Gottman, J. M. (1990). How marriages change. In G.Patterson (Ed.), Depression and aggression in family interaction (pp. 75–102). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
    Gottman, J. M., & Levenson, R. (1986). Assessing the role of emotion in marriage. Behavioral Assessment, 8, 31–48.
    Gottman, J. M., Markman, H., & Notarius, C. (1977). Topography of marital conflict: A sequential analysis of verbal and nonverbal behavior. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 39, 361–377. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/350902
    Gottschalk, L. A., Winget, C. N., & Gleser, G. C. (1969). Manual of instruction for using the Gottschalk-Gleser content analysis scales: Anxiety, hostility, and social alienation-personal disorganization. Berkeley: University of California Press.
    Green, R., & Murray, E. (1973). Instigation to aggression as a function of self-disclosure and threat to self esteem. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 40, 440–443. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0034459
    Groen, J. J. (1975). The measurement of emotion and arousal in the clinical psychological laboratory and in medical practice. In L.Levi (Ed.), Emotions: Their parameters and measurement. New York: Raven.
    Gross, E., & Stone, G. (1964). Embarrassment and the analysis of role requirements. American Journal of Sociology, 70, 1–15. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/223733
    Hall, S. (1899). A study of anger. American Journal of Psychology, 10, 506–591. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/1412662
    Hansburg, H. (1972). Adolescent separation anxiety: A method for the study of adolescent separation problems. Springfield, IL: Charles C Thomas.
    Harlow, H. F. (1962). The heterosexual affectional system in monkeys. American Psychologist, 17, 1–9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0043587
    Harrington, C. L. (1990). Emotion talk: The sequential organization of shame talk. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, Santa Barbara.
    Heritage, J. (1985). Recent developments in conversational analysis. Sociolin-guistics, 15, 1–19.
    Hess, R. D., & Handel, G. (1959). Family worlds: A psychosocial approach to family life. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
    Holtzworth-Munroe, A., & Jacobson, N. S. (1985). Causal attributions of married couples: When do they search for causes? What do they conclude when they do?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48, 1398–1412. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.48.6.1398
    Horowitz, M. J. (1981). Self-righteous rage and the attribution of blame. Archives of General Psychiatry, 38, 1233–1238. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1981.01780360049004
    Izard, C. (1971). The face of emotion. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
    Izard, C. (1977). Human emotion. New York: Plenum.
    Jackins, H. (1965). The human side of human beings. Seattle: Rational Island.
    Jackson, D. D. (1957). The question of family homeostasis. Psychiatric Quarterly Supplement, 31, 79–90.
    Jackson, D. D. (1965a). Family rules: Marital quid pro quo. Archives of General Psychiatry, 12, 589–594. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1965.01720360061010
    Jackson, D. D. (1965b). The study of the family. Family Process, 4, 1–20. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1545-5300.1965.00001.x
    Jacobs, S. (1988). Evidence and inference in conversation analysis. In J. A.Anderson (Ed.), Communication yearbook11 (pp. 433–443). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
    Jacobson, N. S. (1977). Problem solving and contingency in the treatment of marital discord, Journal of Clinical and Consulting Psychology, 45, 92–100. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.45.1.92
    Jacobson, N. S., McDonald, D. W., Follette, W. C., & Berley, R. A. (1985). Attributional processes in distressed and nondistressed married couples. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 9, 35–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01178749
    James, W. (1910). Psychology. New York: Henry Holt.
    Janov, A. (1970). The primal scream. New York: Dell.
    Johnston, J., & Campbell, L. (1988). Impasses in divorce. New York: Free Press.
    JohnstonJ., Campbell, L., & Tall, M. (1984, April 7–11). Impasse to the resolution of custody and visitation disputes. Paper presented at the 61st Annual Meeting of the American Orthopsychiatry Association, Toronto.
    Kahn, M. (1966). The physiology of catharsis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 3, 278–286. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/h0022960
    Kaplin, R. (1975). The cathartic value of self-expression: Testing catharsis, dissonance, and interference explanations. Journal of Social Psychology, 97, 195–208. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00224545.1975.9923339
    Katz, J. (1988). Seductions to crime. New York: Basic Books.
    Kaufman, G. (1989). The psychology of shame: Theory and treatment of shame-based syndromes. New York: Springer.
    Kelley, H. H., Berscheid, E., Christensen, A., Harvey, J. H., Huston, T., Levinger, C., Peplau, L., & Peterson, D. (Eds.). (1983). Close relationships. New York: W. H. Freeman.
    Kendon, A. (1967). Some functions of gaze-direction in social interaction. Acta Psychologica, 29, 22–47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0001-6918%2867%2990005-4
    Kerr, M., & Bowen, M. (1988). Family evaluation. New York: W. W. Norton.
    Klinnert, M., Campos, J., Sorse, J., Emde, R., & Svejda, M. (1982). The development of social referencing in infancy. In R.Plutchick & H.Kellerman (Ed.), Emotion: Theory, research and experience: Vol. 2. Emotion in early development. New York: Academic Press.
    Kohut, H. E. (1971). Thoughts on narcissism and narcissistic rage. In H. E.Kohut, The search for the self. New York: International University Press.
    Kreisberg, L. (1973). The sociology of social conflicts. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
    Labov, W., & Fanshel, D. (1977). Therapeutic discourse. New York: Academic Press.
    Laing, R. D. (1965). The divided self. Baltimore: Penguin.
    Lansky, M. (1980). On blame. International Journal of Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy, 11, 409–425.
    Lansky, M. (1985). Preoccupation as a mode of pathologic distance regulation. International Journal of Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy, 11, 409–425.
    Lansky, M. (1987). Shame and domestic violence. In D.Nathanson (Ed.), The many faces of shame. New York: Guilford.
    Lazare, A. (1979). Unresolved grief. In A.Lazare, Outpatient psychiatry: Diagnosis and treatment. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.
    Lazare, A. (1987). Shame and humiliation in the medical encounter. Archives of Internal Medicine, 147, 1653–1658. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archinte.1987.00370090129021
    Lewis, H. B. (1958). Over-differentiation and under-individuation of the self. Psychoanalysis and the Psychoanalytic Review, 45, 3–24.
    Lewis, H. B. (1971a). Shame and guilt in neurosis. New York: International University Press.
    Lewis, H. B. (1971b). Shame and guilt in neurosis. Psychoanalytic Review, 58, 434–435.
    Lewis, H. B. (1976). Psychic war in men and women. New York: New York University Press.
    Lewis, H. B. (1979). Using content analysis to explore shame and guilt in neurosis. In L. A.Gottschalk (Ed.), The content analysis of verbal behavior. New York: Halstead.
    Lewis, H. B. (1981a). Freud and modern psychology (Vol. 1). New York: Plenum. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4684-3812-3
    Lewis, H. B. (1981b). Shame and guilt in human nature. In S.Tuttman, C.Kaye, & M.Zimmerman (Eds.), Object and self: A developmental approach. New York: International University Press.
    Lewis, H. B. (1983). Freud and modern psychology (Vol. 2). New York: Plenum.
    Lewis, H. B. (1985). Some thoughts on the moral emotions of shame and guilt. In L.Cirillo, B.Kaplin, & S.Wapner (Eds.), Emotions in ideal human development. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
    Lewis, H. B. (Ed.). (1987). The role of shame in symptom formation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
    Lindsay-Hartz, J. (1984). Contrasting experiences of shame and guilt. American Behavioral Scientist, 27, 689–704. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/000276484027006003
    Locke, H. J. (1951). Predicting adjustment in marriage: A comparison of a divorced and a happily married group. New York: Holt.
    Longley, J., & Pruitt, D. (1980). Groupthink: A critique of Janis's theory. Review of Personality and Social Psychology, 1, 74–93.
    Lynd, H. (1958). On shame and the search for identity. New York: Harcourt.
    Mace, D. R. (1976). Marital intimacy and the deadly love-anger cycle. Journal of Marriage and Family Counseling, 2, 131–137. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1752-0606.1976.tb00405.x
    Mahler, M., & McDivitt, J. (1982). Thoughts on the emergence of the sense of self, with particular emphasis on the body self. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 30, 827–848. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/000306518203000401
    Margolin, G., & Wampold, B. E. (1981). A sequential analysis of conflict and accord in distressed and non-distressed marital partners. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49, 554–567. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.49.4.554
    Markman, H. (1981). The prediction of marital distress: A five-year follow-up. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49, 760–762. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-006X.49.5.760
    Markman, H., & Notarius, C. (1987). Coding marital and family interaction: Current status. In T.Jacobs (Ed.), Family interaction and psychopathology: Theories, research and methods (pp. 329–390). New York: Plenum.
    Marx, K. (1964). Economic and philosophic manuscripts of 1844. New York: International Publishers. (Original work published 1844)
    Massie, H. (1982). Affective development and the organization of mother-infant behavior from the perspective of psychopathology. In E.Tronick (Ed.), Social interchange in infancy. Baltimore: University Park Press.
    McDougall, W. (1908). An introduction to social psychology. New York: University Paperbacks. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/12261-000
    Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, self, and society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
    Mead, G. H. (1964). Selected writings. Indianapolis: Bobbs-Merrill.
    Mehrabian, A. (1972). Non-verbal communication. New York: Aldine.
    Mishler, E. (1986). Research interviewing: Context and narrative. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    Mishler, E., & Waxier, N. (1968). Interaction in families. New York: John Wiley.
    Morrison, A. (1989). Shame, the underside of narcissism. Hillsdale, NJ: Analytic Press.
    Morrison, N. (1987). The role of shame in schizophrenia. In H. B.Lewis (Ed.), The role of shame in symptom formation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
    Neuhauser, P. (1988). Tribal warfare in organizations. Cambridge, MA: Ballinger.
    Nichols, M. P., & Zax, M. (1977). Catharsis in psychotherapy. New York: Gardiner.
    Nietzsche, F. (1910). Human, all-too-human. In O.Levy (Ed.), The complete works of Friedrich Nietzsche. London: T. N. Foulis. (Original work published 1878)
    Nietzsche, F. (1954). Beyond good and evil: The philosophy of Nietzsche. New York: Modern Library. (Original work published 1886)
    Nietzsche, F. (1967). Genealogy of morals. New York: Vintage. (Original work published 1887)
    Noller, P. (1984). Nonverbal communication and marital interaction. New York: Pergamon.
    Noller, P., & Fitzpatrick, M. A. (1990). Marital communication in the eighties. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 52. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/353305
    Notarius, C., & Johnson, J. (1982). Emotional expressions in husbands and wives. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 44, 483–489. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/351556
    Notarius, C., & Markman, H. (1981). The Couples Interaction Scoring System. In E. E.Filsinger & R. A.Lewis (Eds.), Observing marriages: New behavioral approaches. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
    Olson, D. (1986). Circumplex model VII: Validation studies and FACES III. Family Process, 25, 337–351. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1545-5300.1986.00337.x
    Olson, D., Lavee, Y., & Cubbin, H. (1988). Types of families and family response to stress across the life cycle. In D.Klein & J.Aldous (Eds.), Social stress and family development (pp. 16–43). New York: Guilford.
    Olson, D., & Rider, R. (1970). Inventory of Marital Conflict (IMC): An experimental interaction procedure. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 32, 443–448. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/350110
    Parsons, T. (1949). The structure of social action. New York: Free Press.
    Parsons, T., & Bales, R. F. (1955). Family, socialization, and interaction process. New York: Free Press.
    Patterson, G. (1982). Coercive family process. Eugene, OR: Castalia.
    Perls, F. (1969). Ego, hunger and aggression. New York: Random House. (Original work published 1947)
    Peterson, D. R. (1979). Assessing interpersonal relationships by means of interaction records. Behavioral Assessment, 1, 221–236. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01321878
    Peterson, D. R. (1983). Conflict. In H.Kelley, E.Berscheid, A.Christensen, J.Harvey, T.Huston, G.Levinger, L.Peplau, & D.Peterson (Eds.), Close relationships. New York: W. H. Freeman.
    Piers, G., & Singer, M. (1953). Shame and guilt. New York: Charles C Thomas.
    Pittenger, R., Hockett, C., & Danehy, J. (1960). The first five minutes. New York: Paul Martineau.
    Pruitt, D. G., & Rubin, J. Z. (1986). Social conflict. New York: Random House.
    Quanty, M. B. (1976). Aggression catharsis: Experimental investigations and implications. In R. G.Geen & E. C.O'Neal (Eds.), Perspectives on aggression. New York: Academic Press.
    Rank, O. (1968). Will, therapy and truth and reality. New York: Knopf. (Original work published 1936)
    Raush, H., Barry, W., Hertel, R., & Swain, M. (1974). Communication, conflict and marriage. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
    Raush, H., Greif, A., & Nugent, J. (1979). Communication in couples and families. In W. R.Burr, R.Hill, F. I.Nye, & I. L.Reiss (Eds.), Contemporary theories about the family (pp. 468–492). New York: Free Press.
    Reiss, D. (1981). The family's construction of reality. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    Retzinger, S. M. (1985). The resentment process: Videotape studies. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 2, 129–151. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0736-9735.2.2.129
    Retzinger, S. M. (1987). Resentment and laughter: Video studies of the shamerage spiral. In H. B.Lewis (Ed.), The role of shame in symptom formation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
    Retzinger, S. M. (1989). A theory of mental illness: Integrating social and emotional aspects. Psychiatry, 52, 325–335.
    Retzinger, S. M. (1991a, Spring). Mental illness and labeling in mediation. Mediation Quarterly.
    Retzinger, S. M. (1991b). The role of shame in marital conflict. Perspectives on Social Problems, 3.
    Retzinger, S. M. (1991c). Shame, anger and conflict: Case study of emotional violence. Journal of Family Violence, 6(1), 37–59. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF00978525
    Rheingold, H. (1969). The social and socializing infant. In D.Goslin (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research. Chicago: Rand McNally.
    Robson, K. S. (1967). The role of eye-to-eye contact in mother-infant attachment. Journal of Psychology and Psychiatry, 8, 13–25. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.1967.tb02176.x
    Roloff, M. E., & Miller, G. R. (Eds.). (1987). Interpersonal process. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
    Rosenstock, F., & Kutner, B. (1967). Alienation and family crisis. Sociological Quarterly, 8, 397–405. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1533-8525.1967.tb01065.x
    Rubin, L. (1983). Intimate strangers: Men and women together. New York: Harper.
    Rubin, T. I. (1970). The angry book. New York: Collier.
    Ruesch, J., & Bateson, G. (1951). Communication: The social matrix of psychiatry. New York: W. W. Norton.
    Saposnek, D. (1983). Mediating child custody disputes. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.
    Satir, V. (1967). Conjoint family therapy. Palo Alto, CA: Science & Behavior.
    Satir, V. (1972). Peoplemaking. Palo Alto, CA: Science & Behavior.
    Scheff, T. J. (1983). Toward integration in the social psychology of emotion. Annual Review of Sociology, 9, 33–54. http://dx.doi.org/10.1146/annurev.so.09.080183.002001
    Scheff, T. J. (1986). Toward resolving the controversy over thick description. Current Anthropology, 27, 408–409. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/203460
    Scheff, T. J. (1987). The shame-rage spiral: A case study of an interminable quarrel. In H. B.Lewis (Ed.), The role of shame in symptom formation. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
    Scheff, T. J. (1988). Shame and conformity: The deference emotion system. American Journal of Sociology, 53, 395–406. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2095647
    Scheff, T. J. (1989). Cognitive and emotional conflict in anorexia: Re-analysis of a case. Psychiatry, 52, 148–161.
    Scheff, T. J. (1990). Microsociology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
    Scheff, T.]., & Retzinger, S. M. (1991). Emotions and violence: Shame and rage in destructive conflicts. Lexington, MA: Free Press.
    Scheff, T.J., Retzinger, S. M., & Ryan, M. (1989). Crime, violence and self-esteem: Review and proposals. In A.Mecca, N.Smelser, & J.Vasconcellos, The social importance of self-esteem. Berkeley: University of California Press.
    Scheflen, A. E. (1960). Regressive one-to-one relationships. Psychiatric Quarterly, 23, 692–709. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/BF01562584
    Scheflen, A. E. (1973). Communicational structure. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
    Scheflen, A. E. (1974). How behavior means. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.
    Scheler, M. (1961). Ressentiment. New York: Free Press.
    Schneider, C. (1977). Shame, exposure and privacy. Boston: Beacon.
    Shane, P. (1980). Shame and learning. American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 50, 348–355. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1939-0025.1980.tb03296.x
    Shaver, K. (1985). The attribution of blame. New York: Springer Verlag. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4612-5094-4
    Shaver, P. (1987). Love and attachment: The integration of three behavioral systems. In R. J.Sternberg & M.Barnes (Eds.), Anatomy of love. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
    Shupe, A., Stacey, W., & Hazelwood, L. (1987). Violent men, violent couples. Lexington, MA: Lexington.
    Sillars, A., Jones, T., & Murphy, M. (1982). Communication and understanding in marriage. Human Communication Research, 10, 317–350. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-2958.1984.tb00022.x
    Sillars, A., & Weisberg, J. (1987). Conflict as a social skill. In M. E.Roloff & G. R.Miller (Eds.), Interpersonal process. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
    Simmel, G. (1955). Conflict and the web of group affiliations. New York: Free Press.
    Sipes, R. G. (1973). Wars, sports and aggression: An empirical analysis of two rival theories. American Anthropologist, 75, 64–86. http://dx.doi.org/10.1525/aa.1973.75.1.02a00040
    Soloviev, V. (1918). The justification of good: An essay on moral philosophy. New York: Macmillan.
    Sorse, J., Emde, R., Campos, J., & Klinnert, M. (1985). Maternal emotional signaling: Its effect on the visual cliff behavior of 1-year-olds. Developmental Psychology, 21, 195–200. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.21.1.195
    Spanier, G. B. (1976). Measuring dyadic adjustment: New scales for measuring the quality of marriages and similar dyads. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 38, 15–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/350547
    Spitz, R. A. (1946). Anaclitic depression: An inquiry into the genesis of psychiatric conditions in early childhood II. Psychoanalytic Study of the Child, 2, 313–342.
    Sprey, J. (1979). Conflict theory and the study of marriage and the family. In W.Burr, R.Hill, F. I.Nye, & I.Reiss (Eds.), Contemporary theories about the family. New York: Free Press.
    Stechler, G., & Carpenter, G. (1967). A viewpoint of early affective development. Exceptional Infant, 1, 165–189.
    Stechler, G., & Latz, E. (1966). Some observations on attention and arousal in the human infant. Journal of Child Psychiatry, 5, 517–525. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0002-7138%2809%2962098-7
    Steiner, G. (1981). After Babel: Aspects of language and translation. New York: Oxford University Press.
    Stern, D. N. (1971). A micro-analysis of mother-infant interaction. Journal of American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 10, 501–517. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0002-7138%2809%2961752-0
    Stern, D. N. (1977). The first relationship. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
    Stern, D. N. (1981). The development of biologically determined signals of readiness to communicate, which are language “resistant.” In R.Stark (Ed.), Language behavior in infancy and early childhood. New York: Elsevier North-Holland.
    Stern, D. N. (1984). Affect attunement: The sharing of feeling states between mother and infant by means of inter-modal fluency. In T.Field & N.Fox (Eds.), Social perception in early infancy. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.
    Straus, M., Gelles, R., & Steinmetz, S. (1980). Behind closed doors: Violence in the American family. Garden City, NY: Doubleday.
    Straus, M., & Hotaling, G. T. (Eds.). (1980). The social causes of husband-wife violence. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
    Strauss, A. (1965). George Herbert Mead on social psychology. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
    Stuart, R. B. (1980). Helping couples change. New York: Guilford.
    Stuart, R. B., & Braver, J. (1973). Positive and negative exchanges between spouses and strangers. Unpublished manuscript.
    Sumner, W. (1906). Folkways. New York: Ginn.
    Tavris, C. (1982). Anger: The misunderstood emotion. New York: Touchstone.
    Terman, L. M., Buttenweiser, P., Ferguson, L., Johnson, W., & Wilson, D. (1938). Psychological factors in marital happiness. New York: McGraw-Hill.
    Terman, L. M., & Wallin, P. (1949). The validity of marriage prediction and marital adjustment tests. American Sociological Review, 14, 503–504. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2087213
    Tomkins, S. (1963). Affect/imagery/consciousness (Vol. 2). New York: Springer.
    Tronick, E. (1980). The primacy of social skills. Exceptional Infant, 4, 144–158.
    Tronick, E., Als, H., & Adamson, L. (1979). Structure of early face-to-face communicative interactions. In A.Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech: The beginning of human communications. New York: Cambridge University Press.
    Tronick, E., Als, H., Adamson, L., Wise, S., & Brazelton, B. (1978). The infants' response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction. American Academy of Child Psychiatry, 17, 1–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/S0002-7138%2809%2962273-1
    Tronick, E., Ricks, M., & Cohn, J. (1982). Maternal and infant affect exchange: Patterns of adaptation. In T.Field & A.Fogel (Eds.), Emotion and early interaction. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
    Tucker, R. C. (Ed.). (1978). The Marx-Engels reader. New York: W. W. Norton.
    Volkema, R. (1988). The mediator as face saver. Mediation Quarterly, 22, 5–14. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/crq.39019882203
    Wallace, L. (1963). The mechanism of shame. Archives of General Psychiatry, 8, 96–101. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.1963.01720070082010
    Walters, R., & Parke, R. (1965). The role of distance receptor in the development of social responsiveness. In L.Lipsitt & C.Spiker (Eds.), Advances in child development and behavior. New York: Academic Press.
    Watzlawick, P., Beavin, J., & Jackson, D. D. (1967). Pragmatics of human communication. New York: W. W. Norton.
    Wurmser, L. (1981). Mask of shame. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Author Index

    About the Author

    Suzanne M. Retzinger received her Ph.D. from the University of California, Santa Barbara, in 1988. She is currently Assistant Research Sociologist at the Community and Organization Research Institute at UCSB, and Family Relations Mediator, Superior Court of Ventura County. She is also conducting a study of protracted child custody disputes in high-conflict families, using case history methods in combination with videotape. She is coauthor, with Thomas Scheff, of Emotions andViolence: Shame and Rage in Destructive Conflicts (Lexington, 1991) and has published articles on conflict, emotions, self-esteem, mental illness, and mediation.

    Notes


    • Loading...
Back to Top

Copy and paste the following HTML into your website